[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Directed Acyclic Graph of the Hypothesized Associations Between Study Variables and Waves of Measurement for the Exposure, Outcome, and Potential Confounders
Directed Acyclic Graph of the Hypothesized Associations Between Study Variables and Waves of Measurement for the Exposure, Outcome, and Potential Confounders

The dashed lines represent potential confounding. The solid line represents the main association of interest. BMI indicates body mass index.

Figure 2.  Adjusted Proportion of Internalizing Problems, Externalizing Problems, and Comorbid Internalizing and Externalizing Problems Stratified by Category of Time Spent on Social Media per Day Among US Adolescents in the Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health Study, 2013-2016
Adjusted Proportion of Internalizing Problems, Externalizing Problems, and Comorbid Internalizing and Externalizing Problems Stratified by Category of Time Spent on Social Media per Day Among US Adolescents in the Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health Study, 2013-2016

Error bars indicate 95% CIs.

Table 1.  Descriptive Statistics of Population Characteristics for US Adolescents in the PATH Study, 2013-2016, Overall and by Internalizing and Externalizing Problemsa
Descriptive Statistics of Population Characteristics for US Adolescents in the PATH Study, 2013-2016, Overall and by Internalizing and Externalizing Problemsa
Table 2.  Unadjusted and Adjusted RRRs for Each Category of Social Media Use Associated With Internalizing and Externalizing Problems Among 6595 US Adolescents in the PATH Study, 2013-2016a
Unadjusted and Adjusted RRRs for Each Category of Social Media Use Associated With Internalizing and Externalizing Problems Among 6595 US Adolescents in the PATH Study, 2013-2016a
Table 3.  Estimated Percentages of Adolescent Mental Health Problem Cases Eliminated in Each Counterfactual Scenario of Time Spent on Social Mediaa
Estimated Percentages of Adolescent Mental Health Problem Cases Eliminated in Each Counterfactual Scenario of Time Spent on Social Mediaa
1.
Pew Research Center. Teens, social media & technology. 2018. https://www.pewinternet.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/9/2018/05/PI_2018.05.31_TeensTech_FINAL.pdf. Accessed April 11, 2019.
2.
Council on Communications and Media.  Media use in school-aged children and adolescents.  Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20162592. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-2592PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Zink  J, Belcher  BR, Kechter  A, Stone  MD, Leventhal  AM.  Reciprocal associations between screen time and emotional disorder symptoms during adolescence.  Prev Med Rep. 2019;13:281-288. doi:10.1016/j.pmedr.2019.01.014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
McCrae  N, Gettings  S, Purssell  E.  Social media and depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review.  Adoles Res Rev. 2017;2(4):315-330. doi:10.1007/s40894-017-0053-4Google Scholar
5.
Primack  BA, Swanier  B, Georgiopoulos  AM, Land  SR, Fine  MJ.  Association between media use in adolescence and depression in young adulthood: a longitudinal study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(2):181-188. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.532PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Toseeb  U, Inkster  B.  Online social networking sites and mental health research.  Front Psychiatry. 2015;6:36.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Jelenchick  LA, Eickhoff  JC, Moreno  MA.  “Facebook depression?” social networking site use and depression in older adolescents.  J Adolesc Health. 2013;52(1):128-130. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Ra  CK, Cho  J, Stone  MD,  et al.  Association of digital media use with subsequent symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents.  JAMA. 2018;320(3):255-263. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.8931PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Galica  VL, Vannucci  A, Flannery  KM, Ohannessian  CM.  Social media use and conduct problems in emerging adults.  Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2017;20(7):448-452. doi:10.1089/cyber.2017.0068PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Mojtabai  R, Olfson  M, Han  B.  National trends in the prevalence and treatment of depression in adolescents and young adults.  Pediatrics. 2016;138(6):e20161878. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-1878PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Twenge  JM, Cooper  AB, Joiner  TE, Duffy  ME, Binau  SG.  Age, period, and cohort trends in mood disorder indicators and suicide-related outcomes in a nationally representative dataset, 2005-2017.  J Abnorm Psychol. 2019;128(3):185-199. doi:10.1037/abn0000410PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Hedegaard  H, Curtin  SC, Warner  M.  Suicide Mortality in the United States, 1999–2017. NCHS Data Brief No. 330. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 2018.
13.
Burstein  B, Agostino  H, Greenfield  B.  Suicidal attempts and ideation among children and adolescents in US emergency departments, 2007-2015 [published online April 8, 2019].  JAMA Pediatr. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.0464PubMedGoogle Scholar
14.
Twenge  JM, Martin  GN, Campbell  WK.  Decreases in psychological well-being among American adolescents after 2012 and links to screen time during the rise of smartphone technology.  Emotion. 2018;18(6):765-780. doi:10.1037/emo0000403PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Hyland  A, Ambrose  BK, Conway  KP,  et al.  Design and methods of the Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health (PATH) Study.  Tob Control. 2017;26(4):371-378. doi:10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2016-052934PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
VanderWeele  TJ, Jackson  JW, Li  S.  Causal inference and longitudinal data: a case study of religion and mental health.  Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(11):1457-1466. doi:10.1007/s00127-016-1281-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
US Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Drug Abuse, Food and Drug Administration Center for Tobacco Products.  Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health (PATH) Study. Ann Arbor, MI: PATH; 2017.
18.
Dennis  ML, Chan  YF, Funk  RR.  Development and validation of the GAIN Short Screener (GSS) for internalizing, externalizing and substance use disorders and crime/violence problems among adolescents and adults.  Am J Addict. 2006;15(suppl 1):80-91. doi:10.1080/10550490601006055PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
McDonell  MG, Comtois  KA, Voss  WD, Morgan  AH, Ries  RK.  Global Appraisal of Individual Needs Short Screener (GSS): psychometric properties and performance as a screening measure in adolescents.  Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2009;35(3):157-160. doi:10.1080/00952990902825421PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Green  VR, Conway  KP, Silveira  ML,  et al.  Mental health problems and onset of tobacco use among 12- to 24-year-olds in the PATH study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;57(12):944-954.e4. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.029PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Riehm  KE, Young  AS, Feder  KA,  et al.  Mental health problems and initiation of e-cigarette and combustible cigarette use.  Pediatrics. 2019;144(1):e20182935.PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Schisterman  EF, Cole  SR, Platt  RW.  Overadjustment bias and unnecessary adjustment in epidemiologic studies.  Epidemiology. 2009;20(4):488-495. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181a819a1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Mantel  N.  The detection of disease clustering and a generalized regression approach.  Cancer Res. 1967;27(2):209-220.PubMedGoogle Scholar
24.
Greenland  S, Drescher  K.  Maximum likelihood estimation of the attributable fraction from logistic models.  Biometrics. 1993;49(3):865-872. doi:10.2307/2532206PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Rückinger  S, von Kries  R, Toschke  AM.  An illustration of and programs estimating attributable fractions in large scale surveys considering multiple risk factors.  BMC Med Res Methodol. 2009;9(1):7. doi:10.1186/1471-2288-9-7PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Tilling  K, Williamson  EJ, Spratt  M, Sterne  JA, Carpenter  JR.  Appropriate inclusion of interactions was needed to avoid bias in multiple imputation.  J Clin Epidemiol. 2016;80:107-115. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2016.07.004PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Li  X, Buxton  OM, Lee  S, Chang  AM, Berger  LM, Hale  L.  Sleep mediates the association between adolescent screen time and depressive symptoms.  Sleep Med. 2019;57:51-60. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2019.01.029PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Bottino  SMB, Bottino  CM, Regina  CG, Correia  AV, Ribeiro  WS.  Cyberbullying and adolescent mental health: systematic review.  Cad Saude Publica. 2015;31(3):463-475. doi:10.1590/0102-311x00036114PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Hoge  E, Bickham  D, Cantor  J.  Digital media, anxiety, and depression in children.  Pediatrics. 2017;140(suppl 2):S76-S80. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-1758GPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Seabrook  EM, Kern  ML, Rickard  NS.  Social networking sites, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review.  JMIR Ment Health. 2016;3(4):e50. doi:10.2196/mental.5842PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Baker  DA, Algorta  GP.  The relationship between online social networking and depression: a systematic review of quantitative studies.  Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2016;19(11):638-648. doi:10.1089/cyber.2016.0206PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Ilakkuvan  V, Johnson  A, Villanti  AC, Evans  WD, Turner  M.  Patterns of social media use and their relationship to health risks among young adults.  J Adolesc Health. 2019;64(2):158-164. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.06.025PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Joshi  SV, Stubbe  D, Li  S-TT, Hilty  DM.  The use of technology by youth: implications for psychiatric educators.  Acad Psychiatry. 2019;43(1):101-109. doi:10.1007/s40596-018-1007-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Jeong  S-H, Cho  H, Hwang  Y.  Media literacy interventions: a meta-analytic review.  J Commun. 2012;62(3):454-472. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01643.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Yurieff  K. Instagram is testing hiding your likes. CNN website. https://www.cnn.com/2019/04/30/tech/instagram-hiding-likes/index.html. Accessed June 12, 2019.
36.
Orben  A, Przybylski  AK.  The association between adolescent well-being and digital technology use.  Nat Hum Behav. 2019;3(2):173-182. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Hunt  MG, Marx  R, Lipson  C,  et al.  No more FOMO: limiting social media decreases loneliness and depression.  J Soc Clin Psychol. 2018;37(10):751-768. doi:10.1521/jscp.2018.37.10.751Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Junco  R.  Comparing actual and self-reported measures of Facebook use.  Comput Human Behav. 2013;29(3):626-631. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.007Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Stier  S, Breuer  J, Siegers  P,  et al.  Integrating survey data and digital trace data: key issues in developing an emerging field  [published online April 24, 2019].  Soc Sci Comput Rev. doi:10.1177/0894439319843669Google Scholar
Original Investigation
September 11, 2019

Associations Between Time Spent Using Social Media and Internalizing and Externalizing Problems Among US Youth

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Mental Health, Bloomberg School of Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland
  • 2Department of Epidemiology, Bloomberg School of Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland
  • 3Welch Center for Prevention, Epidemiology and Clinical Research, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland
  • 5Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland
  • 6Department of Behavioral and Community Health, University of Maryland, College Park, College Park
  • 7Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, North Carolina
  • 8Washington State Department of Health, Olympia
JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76(12):1266-1273. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2325
Key Points

Question  Is time spent using social media associated with mental health problems among adolescents?

Findings  In this cohort study of 6595 US adolescents, increased time spent using social media per day was prospectively associated with increased odds of reporting high levels of internalizing and comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems, even after adjusting for history of mental health problems.

Meaning  Adolescents who spend more than 3 hours per day on social media may be at heightened risk for mental health problems, particularly internalizing problems.

Abstract

Importance  Social media use may be a risk factor for mental health problems in adolescents. However, few longitudinal studies have investigated this association, and none have quantified the proportion of mental health problems among adolescents attributable to social media use.

Objective  To assess whether time spent using social media per day is prospectively associated with internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents.

Design, Setting, and Participants  This longitudinal cohort study of 6595 participants from waves 1 (September 12, 2013, to December 14, 2014), 2 (October 23, 2014, to October 30, 2015), and 3 (October 18, 2015, to October 23, 2016) of the Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health study, a nationally representative cohort study of US adolescents, assessed US adolescents via household interviews using audio computer-assisted self-interviewing. Data analysis was performed from January 14, 2019, to May 22, 2019.

Exposures  Self-reported time spent on social media during a typical day (none, ≤30 minutes, >30 minutes to ≤3 hours, >3 hours to ≤6 hours, and >6 hours) during wave 2.

Main Outcomes and Measure  Self-reported past-year internalizing problems alone, externalizing problems alone, and comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems during wave 3 using the Global Appraisal of Individual Needs–Short Screener.

Results  A total of 6595 adolescents (aged 12-15 years during wave 1; 3400 [51.3%] male) were studied. In unadjusted analyses, spending more than 30 minutes of time on social media, compared with no use, was associated with increased risk of internalizing problems alone (≤30 minutes: relative risk ratio [RRR], 1.30; 95% CI, 0.94-1.78; >30 minutes to ≤3 hours: RRR, 1.89; 95% CI, 1.36-2.64; >3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 2.47; 95% CI, 1.74-3.49; >6 hours: RRR, 2.83; 95% CI, 1.88-4.26) and comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems (≤30 minutes: RRR, 1.39; 95% CI, 1.06-1.82; >30 minutes to ≤3 hours: RRR, 2.34; 95% CI, 1.83-3.00; >3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 3.15; 95% CI, 2.43-4.09; >6 hours: RRR, 4.29; 95% CI, 3.22-5.73); associations with externalizing problems were inconsistent. In adjusted analyses, use of social media for more than 3 hours per day compared with no use remained significantly associated with internalizing problems alone (>3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 1.60; 95% CI, 1.11-2.31; >6 hours: RRR, 1.78; 95% CI, 1.15-2.77) and comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems (>3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 2.01; 95% CI, 1.51-2.66; >6 hours: RRR, 2.44; 95% CI, 1.73-3.43) but not externalizing problems alone.

Conclusions and Relevance  Adolescents who spend more than 3 hours per day using social media may be at heightened risk for mental health problems, particularly internalizing problems. Future research should determine whether setting limits on daily social media use, increasing media literacy, and redesigning social media platforms are effective means of reducing the burden of mental health problems in this population.

Introduction

For adolescents in the United States, social media use is ubiquitous. A 2018 Pew Research Center poll found that 97% of adolescents report using at least 1 of the 7 most popular social media platforms (YouTube, Instagram, Snapchat, Facebook, Twitter, Tumblr, and Reddit). Moreover, digital media use by adolescents is common: 95% report owning or having access to a smartphone, and almost 90% report they are online at least several times a day.1

Social media offers numerous potential benefits to users, including exposure to current events, interpersonal connection, and enhancement of social support networks.2 However, concerns are increasingly raised about potential harms of social media use.2 One-quarter of adolescents think social media has a mostly negative influence on people their age, pointing to reasons like rumor spreading, lack of in-person contact, unrealistic views of others’ lives, peer pressure, and mental health issues.1

An increasing body of literature suggests that social media use is associated with mental health problems in adolescence. Numerous cross-sectional studies and a limited number of longitudinal studies suggest that high levels of social media use are associated with internalizing problems, including depressive and anxiety symptoms,3-6 although results are not entirely consistent.7 Some studies also suggest an association between social media use and externalizing problems, such as bullying and attention problems.8,9 Furthermore, a previous study4 produced mixed results regarding the possible moderating effect of sex.

The prevalence of major depressive disorder and depressive symptoms has increased among adolescents in the United States,10,11 and adolescent suicide death and attempt rates have increased sharply during the past 2 decades.12,13 Some authors14 have postulated that increases in depression may be attributable to rapid increases in social media use. However, evidence of this association in nationally representative samples is scarce, and little is known about whether reducing time spent on social media might influence the prevalence of mental health problems at a national level.

In this article, we build on existing literature by examining the prospective association of time spent on social media with internalizing and externalizing problems in a representative sample of US adolescents. We used data from the Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health (PATH) study, which is a nationally representative, longitudinal cohort of adolescents.15 Unlike a prior study,16 we adjusted for mental health problems measured before the exposure, which is critical for reducing the influence of reverse causality. We hypothesized that greater time spent on social media would prospectively be associated with internalizing and externalizing problems alone, as well as comorbid problems at 1-year follow-up. On the basis of past research,5 we also examined whether these associations differed between males and females.

Methods
Participants

In this longitudinal cohort study, participants were drawn from the public-use data files of waves 1 (September 12, 2013, to December 14, 2014), 2 (October 23, 2014, to October 30, 2015), and 3 (October 18, 2015, to October 23, 2016) of the PATH study.15 The methods of the PATH study have been previously described.15 In brief, the target population for this survey was the civilian household population in the United States. Data were collected in 1-year intervals, starting with wave 1 from September 12, 2013, to December 14, 2014. Multistage-stratified sampling was used to obtain a sample of households from which up to 2 individuals aged 12 to 17 years were randomly selected to be interviewed. Data analysis was performed from January 14, 2019, to May 22, 2019. After oral parent permission and adolescent assent were obtained, adolescents were interviewed using audio computer-assisted self-interviewing. The current analyses were considered exempt from human subjects research according to Johns Hopkins institutional review board policy because the data were publicly available and deidentified.

The weighted response rate for adolescents during wave 1 was 78.4%, and the weighted retention rate during wave 3 was 83.3%.17 A total of 7595 adolescents (aged 12-15 years during wave 1, aged 13-16 years during wave 2, and aged 14-17 years during wave 3) completed all 3 PATH survey waves. Of these, 1000 adolescents (13.2%) were excluded because they were missing data on at least 1 variable required for this analysis; the remaining 6595 adolescents comprised the analytic sample (eFigure in the Supplement).

Measures
Outcome (Wave 3)

Past-year mental health problems, the outcome of interest, were assessed during wave 3 using the Global Appraisal of Individual Needs–Short Screener (GAIN-SS).18 The GAIN-SS is a screening measure intended to identify a probable mental health disorder and assess symptom severity; it has been validated in adolescents19 and includes internalizing and externalizing subscales (eTable 1 in the Supplement). Each item measures 1 symptom; for this study, symptoms were considered to be present if the respondent selected in the past month or 2 to 12 months from the response options that indicated the last time they had experienced that symptom. Symptom counts were generated for each subscale. Adolescents were classified as reporting low to moderate (0-3 symptoms) or high (≥4 symptoms) internalizing and externalizing problems. These cut points have been validated for use when making treatment decisions18 and have previously been used with the PATH sample.20,21 We combined these subscales to create a single outcome variable with 4 mutually exclusive categories: no or low internalizing and externalizing problems, internalizing problems alone, externalizing problems alone, and comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems. Comorbid problems were defined as having all 4 internalizing and 4 or more externalizing symptoms.

Exposure (Wave 2)

The exposure of interest was time spent using social media per day during wave 2. Adolescents who reported that they ever went online were asked, “Sometimes people use the internet to connect with other people online through social networks like Facebook, Google Plus, YouTube, MySpace, Linkedin, Twitter, Tumblr, Instagram, Pinterest, or Snapchat. This is often called ‘social media.’ Do you have a social media account?” Adolescents who reported that they had a social media account that they visited were asked, “On a typical day, about how much total time do you spend on social media sites?” The response options were up to 30 minutes; more than 30 minutes, up to 3 hours; more than 3 hours, up to 6 hours; and more than 6 hours. We retained these categories for our exposure variable, with an additional category of none for adolescents who reported not going online, not having a social media account, or never visiting their social media account.

Covariates (Wave 1)

Potential confounders, including demographic characteristics (ie, sex, age, race, and parental educational level), body mass index (based on parent-reported weight and height), self-reported lifetime marijuana use and alcohol use, and scale scores for lifetime internalizing and externalizing problems, were adjusted for in the analyses. To ensure that we did not improperly adjust for mediating variables,22 we used covariates measured at wave 1 instead of wave 2. The full study design is displayed in Figure 1.

Statistical Analysis

Multinomial logistic regression was used to estimate the associations between time spent on social media per day with internalizing problems alone, externalizing problems alone, and comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems (reference group: no or low internalizing and externalizing problems). Both unadjusted and adjusted analyses were conducted. Regression coefficients were exponentiated for interpretation as relative risk ratios (RRRs). In addition, we used the adjusted model to generate and plot predicted probabilities of high internalizing and externalizing problems for each level of social media use for an otherwise average study participant.

We tested for the presence of a linear trend in the coefficients for social media use in their relation to each category of mental health problems by converting the social media use variable to an ordinal variable and reestimating the adjusted model (ie, a Mantel test for trend23). A linear trend would suggest that more time spent on social media is associated with a proportionally greater likelihood of reporting mental health problems.

We tested whether any observed association of social media use with mental health problems differed between males and females by testing an interaction term between social media use and sex in our adjusted model.

In addition, we estimated the respective proportions of high internalizing and high externalizing problem cases that would be potentially prevented if adolescents spent less time using social media (ie, the population-attributable fraction [PAF] for social media use). We did this for 4 counterfactual scenarios that represented increasingly greater population reductions in social media use. In scenario 1, adolescents who actually used social media more than 6 hours per day would instead use social media more than 3 hours to 6 hours or less per day; in scenario 2, adolescents who actually used social media more than 3 hours per day would instead use social media more than 30 minutes to 3 hours or less per day; in scenario 3, adolescents who actually used social media more than 30 minutes per day would instead use social media 30 minutes or less per day; and in scenario 4, adolescents who actually spent any amount of time on social media per day would instead not spend any time on social media.

We estimated each scenario by generating a counterfactual population from our adjusted model using the approach to calculate PAFs described by Greenland and Drescher24 and Rückinger et al.25 See the eMethods in the Supplement for a detailed description.

To test whether our results were sensitive to missing data, we repeated analyses using multiply imputed data. We performed multiple imputation using chained equations and recomputed the unadjusted, adjusted, and sex-interaction models. We stratified by sex and generated 10 imputed data sets to account for the hypothesized interaction between sex and social media use.26

Data for analyses were weighted to be representative of 12- to 15-year-old adolescents living in the United States in 2013 to 2014. Standard errors were estimated using the wave 3 all-waves replicate weights constructed using balanced repeated replication (the Fay method) provided in the PATH data set. Statistical significance was assessed at a 2-sided P < .05 level. All analyses were conducted using Stata, version 14 (StataCorp).

Results
Sample Characteristics

A total of 6595 adolescents (aged 12-15 years during wave 1; 3400 [51.3%] male) were included in the analysis. During wave 3, of the sample of 6595 adolescents, 611 (9.1%) reported internalizing problems alone, 885 (14.0%) reported externalizing problems alone, 1169 (17.7%) reported comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems, and the remaining 3930 (59.3%) reported no or low problems. During wave 2, a total of 1125 adolescents (16.8%) reported no social media use, 2082 (31.8%) reported 30 minutes or less, 2000 (30.7%) reported more than 30 minutes to 3 hours or more, 817 (12.3%) reported more than 3 hours to 6 hours or less, and 571 (8.4%) reported more than 6 hours of use per day. Sample characteristics are given in Table 1.

Association Between Social Media Use and Mental Health Problems

Compared with adolescents who did not use social media, the use of social media for more than 30 minutes per day was associated with greater risk of internalizing problems alone (≤30 minutes: RRR, 1.30; 95% CI, 0.94-1.78; >30 minutes to ≤3 hours: RRR, 1.89; 95% CI, 1.36-2.64; >3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 2.47; 95% CI, 1.74-3.49; >6 hours: RRR, 2.83; 95% CI, 1.88-4.26) and comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems (≤30 minutes: RRR, 1.39; 95% CI, 1.06-1.82; >30 minutes to ≤3 hours: RRR, 2.34; 95% CI, 1.83-3.00; >3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 3.15; 95% CI, 2.43-4.09; >6 hours: RRR, 4.29; 95% CI, 3.22-5.73) (Table 2). In the adjusted model, the associations for the 2 highest categories of social media use persisted for internalizing problems alone (>3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 1.60; 95% CI, 1.11-2.31; >6 hours: RRR, 1.78; 95% CI, 1.15-2.77), and the associations for the 3 highest categories of social media use persisted for comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems (>30 minutes to ≤3 hours: RRR, 1.59; 95% CI, 1.23-2.05; >3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 2.01; 95% CI, 1.51-2.66; >6 hours: RRR, 2.44; 95% CI, 1.73-3.43). In contrast, in unadjusted analyses, the association of social media use with externalizing problems was inconsistent (≤30 minutes: RRR, 1.28; 95% CI, 0.98-1.67; >30 minutes to ≤3 hours: RRR, 1.60; 95% CI, 1.16-2.21; >3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 1.36; 95% CI, 0.97-1.90; >6 hours: RRR, 1.59; 95% CI, 1.07-2.37) and not significant in the adjusted analysis (≤30 minutes: RRR, 1.18; 95% CI, 0.89-1.56; >30 minutes to ≤3 hours: RRR, 1.37; 95% CI, 0.98-1.92; >3 to ≤6 hours: RRR, 1.22; 95% CI, 0.86-1.72; >6 hours: RRR, 1.40; 95% CI, 0.90-2.19) (Table 2). The predicted probabilities of high internalizing, externalizing, and comorbid problems for each level of social media use, with all other covariates set to their mean, are displayed in Figure 2.

We observed a significant linear trend in the coefficients for both internalizing (F1,99 = 8.86, P = .004) and comorbid problems (F1,99 = 35.16, P < .001); as time on social media increased, the odds of these outcomes increased proportionately. In contrast, we observed no association for externalizing problems (F1,99 = 2.25, P = .14).

We observed no statistically significant interaction between social media use and sex for internalizing (F4,96 = 0.84, P = .50), externalizing (F4,96 = 0.32, P = .86), or comorbid problems (F4,96 = 0.73, P = .57).

All PAF estimates are given in Table 3. On the basis of our adjusted model assuming no confounding, 0.8% to 18.9% of internalizing problems and 0.8% to 15.3% of externalizing problems could be prevented if participants had instead used less social media.

Results of analyses using multiple imputation methods did not differ appreciably from the main analyses (eTable 2 in the Supplement).

Discussion

Consistent with a prior study,4 we found that adolescent social media use was prospectively associated with increased risk of comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems as well as internalizing problems alone. This association remained significant after adjusting for demographics, past alcohol and marijuana use, and, most importantly, a history of mental health problems, which mitigates the possibility that reverse causality explains these findings. In contrast, we did not find an association of social media use with externalizing problems alone. This finding suggests that the association of social media use with comorbid problems occurs primarily because of the association of social media with internalizing problems and the high comorbidity of internalizing and externalizing problems. Unlike a prior study,4 we found no evidence of moderation by sex, perhaps because of the simplicity of our social media use variable, which could not capture the nature of interactions on social media that may differ by sex.

Numerous mechanisms could account for the association between social media use and internalizing problems. Adolescents who engage in high levels of social media use may experience poorer quality sleep, which may be a mediator on the pathway to internalizing problems.27 Time spent on social media may increase the risk of experiencing cyberbullying, which has a strong association with depressive symptoms.28 Social media may also expose adolescents to idealized self-presentations that negatively influence body image and encourage social comparisons.4 Poor emotion regulation and lack of social interaction may also be associated with social media use and contribute to symptoms of anxiety and depression.29

These mechanisms are potentially consistent with the notion that spending less time on social media may contribute to mental health. In fact, the PAFs obtained in our study suggest that if adolescents using social media for more than 30 minutes per day had instead used it for 30 minutes or less, there would have been 9.4% fewer high internalizing problem cases and 7.3% fewer high externalizing problem cases. Of importance, this is not meant to imply that reductions in mental health problems would definitively happen if social media use were reduced or that all social media use is harmful. Instead, these PAFs suggest the potential influence of our findings on the population at a national level assuming a causal effect of social media use and no confounding—both strong assumptions. Future research could improve on our PAF estimates by using data from randomized clinical trials (RCTs).

Our findings must be balanced with the potential benefits of social media use, which include exposure to current events, communication over geographic barriers, and social inclusion for those who may be otherwise excluded in their day-to-day lives (eg, lesbian, bisexual, transgender, queer, and questioning youth).2 A limitation of our study is that we measured overall time spent on social media; prior studies30-32 have found that social media use may be positively or negatively associated with mental health depending on which platforms are used and how. Nevertheless, a number of interventions could lead to a reduction in time spent on social media by adolescents, while still allowing for the benefits of such use. The American Academy of Pediatrics has developed a Family Media Use Plan, which can be tailored to specific developmental phases and help parents set reasonable rules for digital media use.2 Pediatricians and teachers are essential for promoting these plans, as well as helping parents identify problematic social media use in their children.33 There is also evidence that interventions that promote media literacy, defined as “specific knowledge and skills that can help critical understanding and usage of the media,”34(p 455) counteract the harmful association of media use with behavioral health.34 Also, there is an increasing movement to improve the design of social media platforms; a notable recent example is not displaying the number of “likes” that an Instagram post receives.35 We believe that technology companies and regulators responsible for social media platforms should consider how these platforms can be designed to minimize risk of mental health problems.

Some researchers have raised concerns that studies on technology use and well-being are limited by publication bias.36 We believe that this is a legitimate concern given that many studies on this topic, including the present study, are secondary analyses of data not collected for the purpose of studying social media.36 There appears to be an urgent need for experimental research, specifically a priori registered RCTs that examine interventions designed to reduce social media use. Our study findings suggest a population-level association between social media use and mental health problems, and evidence from RCTs could build on this by examining changes in mental health as a result of changes in social media use. The existing observational study findings and at least 1 RCT in college students37 appear to be sufficient to justify investment in these trials. In addition, RCTs may be valuable for developing clinical guidelines and informing regulatory policy for social media design.

Limitations

Some limitations of this study should be noted. First, adolescents self-reported the exposure and outcome, which may inflate the observed associations. Second, we measured mental health problems with a self-report questionnaire rather than a diagnostic interview. Third, the validity of self-reported time spent on social media in the PATH study is unknown. Some research suggests that self-reported time on social media may exceed actual use38; future studies should consider the use of digital trace data to capture actual time spent using social media.39 Fourth, social media use continues to change rapidly over time; although our data were collected relatively recently, they may not reflect current trends. Fifth, although our study design mitigates the possibility of reverse causality, some residual confounding from imprecise measurement of prior mental health problems may have been present. Sixth, it remains possible that mental health problems are prospectively associated with social media use, but we could not examine this in the present study because of data limitations. Seventh, it is possible that the observed associations were an artifact of unmeasured confounding. Although we controlled for a number of potential confounders, there may be others, such as physical activity, that we were unable to include because of data limitations.

Conclusions

This study suggests that increased time spent on social media may be a risk factor for internalizing problems in adolescents. Future research should determine whether setting limits on daily social media use, increasing media literacy, and redesigning social media platforms are effective means of reducing the burden of mental health problems in this population.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: June 14, 2019.

Corresponding Author: Kira E. Riehm, MS, Department of Mental Health, Bloomberg School of Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, 624 N Broadway, Baltimore, MD 21205 ([email protected]).

Published Online: September 11, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2325

Author Contributions: Ms Riehm had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Riehm, Feder, Crum, Green, La Flair, Mojtabai.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Riehm, Feder, Tormohlen, Young, Green, Pacek, La Flair.

Drafting of the manuscript: Riehm, Feder, Pacek.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

Statistical analysis: Riehm, Feder, Green, Pacek.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Green.

Supervision: Crum, Green, Mojtabai.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Young reported receiving grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse and the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation during the conduct of the study, receiving grants from Supernus Pharmaceuticals and Psychnostics LLC outside the submitted work, and receiving personal fees from University of Montana's American Indian/Alaska Native Clinical Translational Program. Dr Pacek reported receiving grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse during the conduct of the study. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: Ms Riehm was supported by grant 5T32MH014592-39 from the National Institute of Mental Health Psychiatric Epidemiology Training Program (Peter Zandi, principal investigator) and by a doctoral foreign study award from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. Dr Feder was supported by National Research and Service Award F31DA044699 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. Ms Tormohlen was supported by grant T32DA007292 (Renee M. Johnson, principal investigator), Dr Young was supported by grant K23DA044288, and Dr Pacek was supported by grant K01DA043413 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References
1.
Pew Research Center. Teens, social media & technology. 2018. https://www.pewinternet.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/9/2018/05/PI_2018.05.31_TeensTech_FINAL.pdf. Accessed April 11, 2019.
2.
Council on Communications and Media.  Media use in school-aged children and adolescents.  Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20162592. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-2592PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Zink  J, Belcher  BR, Kechter  A, Stone  MD, Leventhal  AM.  Reciprocal associations between screen time and emotional disorder symptoms during adolescence.  Prev Med Rep. 2019;13:281-288. doi:10.1016/j.pmedr.2019.01.014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
McCrae  N, Gettings  S, Purssell  E.  Social media and depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review.  Adoles Res Rev. 2017;2(4):315-330. doi:10.1007/s40894-017-0053-4Google Scholar
5.
Primack  BA, Swanier  B, Georgiopoulos  AM, Land  SR, Fine  MJ.  Association between media use in adolescence and depression in young adulthood: a longitudinal study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(2):181-188. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.532PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Toseeb  U, Inkster  B.  Online social networking sites and mental health research.  Front Psychiatry. 2015;6:36.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Jelenchick  LA, Eickhoff  JC, Moreno  MA.  “Facebook depression?” social networking site use and depression in older adolescents.  J Adolesc Health. 2013;52(1):128-130. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Ra  CK, Cho  J, Stone  MD,  et al.  Association of digital media use with subsequent symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents.  JAMA. 2018;320(3):255-263. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.8931PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Galica  VL, Vannucci  A, Flannery  KM, Ohannessian  CM.  Social media use and conduct problems in emerging adults.  Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2017;20(7):448-452. doi:10.1089/cyber.2017.0068PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Mojtabai  R, Olfson  M, Han  B.  National trends in the prevalence and treatment of depression in adolescents and young adults.  Pediatrics. 2016;138(6):e20161878. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-1878PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Twenge  JM, Cooper  AB, Joiner  TE, Duffy  ME, Binau  SG.  Age, period, and cohort trends in mood disorder indicators and suicide-related outcomes in a nationally representative dataset, 2005-2017.  J Abnorm Psychol. 2019;128(3):185-199. doi:10.1037/abn0000410PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Hedegaard  H, Curtin  SC, Warner  M.  Suicide Mortality in the United States, 1999–2017. NCHS Data Brief No. 330. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 2018.
13.
Burstein  B, Agostino  H, Greenfield  B.  Suicidal attempts and ideation among children and adolescents in US emergency departments, 2007-2015 [published online April 8, 2019].  JAMA Pediatr. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.0464PubMedGoogle Scholar
14.
Twenge  JM, Martin  GN, Campbell  WK.  Decreases in psychological well-being among American adolescents after 2012 and links to screen time during the rise of smartphone technology.  Emotion. 2018;18(6):765-780. doi:10.1037/emo0000403PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Hyland  A, Ambrose  BK, Conway  KP,  et al.  Design and methods of the Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health (PATH) Study.  Tob Control. 2017;26(4):371-378. doi:10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2016-052934PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
VanderWeele  TJ, Jackson  JW, Li  S.  Causal inference and longitudinal data: a case study of religion and mental health.  Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(11):1457-1466. doi:10.1007/s00127-016-1281-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
US Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Drug Abuse, Food and Drug Administration Center for Tobacco Products.  Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health (PATH) Study. Ann Arbor, MI: PATH; 2017.
18.
Dennis  ML, Chan  YF, Funk  RR.  Development and validation of the GAIN Short Screener (GSS) for internalizing, externalizing and substance use disorders and crime/violence problems among adolescents and adults.  Am J Addict. 2006;15(suppl 1):80-91. doi:10.1080/10550490601006055PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
McDonell  MG, Comtois  KA, Voss  WD, Morgan  AH, Ries  RK.  Global Appraisal of Individual Needs Short Screener (GSS): psychometric properties and performance as a screening measure in adolescents.  Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2009;35(3):157-160. doi:10.1080/00952990902825421PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Green  VR, Conway  KP, Silveira  ML,  et al.  Mental health problems and onset of tobacco use among 12- to 24-year-olds in the PATH study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;57(12):944-954.e4. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.029PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Riehm  KE, Young  AS, Feder  KA,  et al.  Mental health problems and initiation of e-cigarette and combustible cigarette use.  Pediatrics. 2019;144(1):e20182935.PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Schisterman  EF, Cole  SR, Platt  RW.  Overadjustment bias and unnecessary adjustment in epidemiologic studies.  Epidemiology. 2009;20(4):488-495. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181a819a1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Mantel  N.  The detection of disease clustering and a generalized regression approach.  Cancer Res. 1967;27(2):209-220.PubMedGoogle Scholar
24.
Greenland  S, Drescher  K.  Maximum likelihood estimation of the attributable fraction from logistic models.  Biometrics. 1993;49(3):865-872. doi:10.2307/2532206PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Rückinger  S, von Kries  R, Toschke  AM.  An illustration of and programs estimating attributable fractions in large scale surveys considering multiple risk factors.  BMC Med Res Methodol. 2009;9(1):7. doi:10.1186/1471-2288-9-7PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Tilling  K, Williamson  EJ, Spratt  M, Sterne  JA, Carpenter  JR.  Appropriate inclusion of interactions was needed to avoid bias in multiple imputation.  J Clin Epidemiol. 2016;80:107-115. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2016.07.004PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Li  X, Buxton  OM, Lee  S, Chang  AM, Berger  LM, Hale  L.  Sleep mediates the association between adolescent screen time and depressive symptoms.  Sleep Med. 2019;57:51-60. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2019.01.029PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Bottino  SMB, Bottino  CM, Regina  CG, Correia  AV, Ribeiro  WS.  Cyberbullying and adolescent mental health: systematic review.  Cad Saude Publica. 2015;31(3):463-475. doi:10.1590/0102-311x00036114PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Hoge  E, Bickham  D, Cantor  J.  Digital media, anxiety, and depression in children.  Pediatrics. 2017;140(suppl 2):S76-S80. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-1758GPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Seabrook  EM, Kern  ML, Rickard  NS.  Social networking sites, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review.  JMIR Ment Health. 2016;3(4):e50. doi:10.2196/mental.5842PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Baker  DA, Algorta  GP.  The relationship between online social networking and depression: a systematic review of quantitative studies.  Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2016;19(11):638-648. doi:10.1089/cyber.2016.0206PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Ilakkuvan  V, Johnson  A, Villanti  AC, Evans  WD, Turner  M.  Patterns of social media use and their relationship to health risks among young adults.  J Adolesc Health. 2019;64(2):158-164. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.06.025PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Joshi  SV, Stubbe  D, Li  S-TT, Hilty  DM.  The use of technology by youth: implications for psychiatric educators.  Acad Psychiatry. 2019;43(1):101-109. doi:10.1007/s40596-018-1007-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Jeong  S-H, Cho  H, Hwang  Y.  Media literacy interventions: a meta-analytic review.  J Commun. 2012;62(3):454-472. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01643.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Yurieff  K. Instagram is testing hiding your likes. CNN website. https://www.cnn.com/2019/04/30/tech/instagram-hiding-likes/index.html. Accessed June 12, 2019.
36.
Orben  A, Przybylski  AK.  The association between adolescent well-being and digital technology use.  Nat Hum Behav. 2019;3(2):173-182. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Hunt  MG, Marx  R, Lipson  C,  et al.  No more FOMO: limiting social media decreases loneliness and depression.  J Soc Clin Psychol. 2018;37(10):751-768. doi:10.1521/jscp.2018.37.10.751Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Junco  R.  Comparing actual and self-reported measures of Facebook use.  Comput Human Behav. 2013;29(3):626-631. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.007Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Stier  S, Breuer  J, Siegers  P,  et al.  Integrating survey data and digital trace data: key issues in developing an emerging field  [published online April 24, 2019].  Soc Sci Comput Rev. doi:10.1177/0894439319843669Google Scholar
×